91麻豆天美直播

Skip to main content

鈥淪econd-Guessing鈥 is a Hard-Wired Behavior, Study Suggests

Media Contact:

Julie Kiefer
Director, Research Communications, 91麻豆天美直播
Email: Julie.Kiefer@hsc.utah.edu

Have you ever made a decision that, in hindsight, seemed irrational? A new study with mice, which could have implications for people, suggests that some decisions are, to a certain extent, beyond their control. Rather, the mice are hard-wired to make them.

鈥淭his research is telling us that animals are constrained in the decisions they make,鈥 says , a neurobiologist at 91麻豆天美直播 and senior author of the study that recently published in . 鈥淭heir genetics push them down one path or another.鈥

Gregg and fellow U of U Health neurobiologist had noticed that mice repeatedly made what appeared to be an irrational decision. After finding a stash of hidden seeds, rather than staying put to eat them, mice kept returning to a location that had food in it the day before. Only on this day, the original location was empty.

鈥淚t was as if the mice were second-guessing whether the first location really had no food,鈥 Gregg says. 鈥淟ike they thought they had missed something.鈥

Mouse deciding whether to take the path to "doubt" or the path to "reward."
U of U Health scientists have found that genes bias decision-making, even decisions that seem irrational. Credit: Cornelia Stacher-H枚rndli, PhD.

To Gregg, Shepherd, and the study鈥檚 co-authors, the behavior didn鈥檛 make any sense. The animals ended up eating less because of the time spent continuously returning to the empty food patch. If that kind of behavior causes mice to eat less in the wild, it could spell trouble, Gregg explains, because not getting enough calories can be detrimental for a mouse.

The real surprise came after discovering that mice lacking a specific gene didn鈥檛 鈥渟econd-guess鈥 where to go and instead were more likely to stay and eat the food they found. As a result, they consumed more calories overall. This was the first evidence the scientists found that genes could bias decision-making, even decisions that did not seem logical, at least to a human. In this case, the gene Arc appeared to be important for compelling the mice to continue searching for food even when it didn鈥檛 appear to be necessary.

"We all have a clear sense of what it is like to second-guess something, but who would have thought that this type of behavior could be so profoundly affected by one gene?"

鈥淲e all have a clear sense of what it is like to second-guess something, but who would have thought that this type of behavior could be so profoundly affected by one gene?鈥 says Cornelia Stacher- H枚rndli, PhD, who was the study's co-first author with Alicia Ravens, a graduate student in Shepherd's lab. 鈥淭his raises the question, are other cognitive biases under genetic control?鈥

Decoding behavior

To the human eye, a mouse鈥檚 life seems pretty simple. When placed in a naturalistic setting in Gregg鈥檚 lab, they left home, explored their surroundings, searched for food, ate a little, and made stops back home in between. But the view looked quite different after a machine learning algorithm deconstructed their journeys.

A custom program built by Gregg and study co-author Jared Emery analyzed 1,609 foraging excursions and saw that the mice repeated 24 behavior sequences over and over. As the mice foraged, they strung together the sequences like building blocks, interspersing them with spontaneous behaviors to construct more complex behavior patterns. One of them was the second-guessing behavior. 鈥淭o a certain extent, you could predict the future,鈥 Gregg remarks.

Building blocks with map and mouse walking towards cheese drawn on them
Machine learning algorithms showed that as mice foraged for food, they strung together a finite number of stereotyped behavioral sequences like building blocks, interspersing them with spontaneous behaviors to construct more complex behavior patterns. Credit: Cornelia Stacher-H枚rndli, PhD.

That future changed for mice missing the gene, Arc. Six of the 24 behavior sequences were altered, and together, those differences short-circuited the second-guessing behavior. Previous research by Shepherd had shown Arc is involved in learning and memory. But overall, analysis showed that the mice鈥檚 memory鈥攁nd their other behaviors鈥攚ere largely intact. "The mice remembered where the food had been originally located, and that was a bit of a surprise," Shepherd says.

The implication is that the effect on those six behaviors was specific.

鈥淥ne intriguing idea is that the animals evolved to make those decisions because they were somehow advantageous in the wild,鈥 Gregg says. He explains one possibility: when mice go back and forth to evaluate previous food locations, it helps them create a mental map. And that might help them find food faster the next time around. 鈥淕enetically controlled cognitive bias may allow for effective decision-making during foraging,鈥 he says.

The question remains, is there a biological basis for other types of cognitive bias? And could genes guide decision-making in humans? More research will tell. 鈥淚 believe that this research is foundational for a new field that we are calling 鈥榙ecision genetics,鈥欌 Stacher-H枚rndli says.

# # #

In addition to Gregg and Stacher-H枚rndli, additional co-authors are Alicia Ravens, Susan Steinward, and Jason Shepherd from 91麻豆天美直播 and Jared Emery from Storyline Health, Inc.

The research was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health and published as 鈥Arc Regulates a Second-Guessing Cognitive Bias During Naturalistic Foraging Through Effects on Discrete Behavior Modules.鈥

C.G. is a co-founder of and has equity in Storyline Health Inc., which uses artificial intelligence to build scalable research and clinical tools for precision medicine, and has advisory roles in Rubicon A.I., DepoIQ, and Uncharted Health. J.E. is an employee of Storyline Health.